Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Articles
    • Current
    • Early Release
    • Archive
    • Rufus A. Lyman Award
    • Theme Issues
    • Special Collections
  • Authors
    • Author Instructions
    • Submission Process
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Call for Papers - Intersectionality of Pharmacists’ Professional and Personal Identity
  • Reviewers
    • Reviewer Instructions
    • Call for Mentees
    • Reviewer Recognition
    • Frequently Asked Questions (FAQ)
  • About
    • About AJPE
    • Editorial Team
    • Editorial Board
    • History
  • More
    • Meet the Editors
    • Webinars
    • Contact AJPE
  • Other Publications

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education
  • Other Publications
American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education

Advanced Search

  • Articles
    • Current
    • Early Release
    • Archive
    • Rufus A. Lyman Award
    • Theme Issues
    • Special Collections
  • Authors
    • Author Instructions
    • Submission Process
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • Call for Papers - Intersectionality of Pharmacists’ Professional and Personal Identity
  • Reviewers
    • Reviewer Instructions
    • Call for Mentees
    • Reviewer Recognition
    • Frequently Asked Questions (FAQ)
  • About
    • About AJPE
    • Editorial Team
    • Editorial Board
    • History
  • More
    • Meet the Editors
    • Webinars
    • Contact AJPE
  • Follow AJPE on Twitter
  • LinkedIn
Research ArticleRESEARCH

Correlation Between Pharmacy Students’ Implicit Bias Scores, Explicit Bias Scores, and Responses to Clinical Cases

Jennifer Santee, Kylie Barnes, Nancy Borja-Hart, An-Lin Cheng, Juanita Draime, Akesha Edwards, Nkem Nonyel and Mark Sawkin
American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education January 2022, 86 (1) 8587; DOI: https://doi.org/10.5688/ajpe8587
Jennifer Santee
aUniversity of Missouri – Kansas City, School of Pharmacy, Kansas City, Missouri
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kylie Barnes
aUniversity of Missouri – Kansas City, School of Pharmacy, Kansas City, Missouri
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nancy Borja-Hart
bUniversity of Tennessee Health Science Center, College of Pharmacy, Memphis, Tennessee
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
An-Lin Cheng
cUniversity of Missouri – Kansas City, School of Medicine, Kansas City, Missouri
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Juanita Draime
dCedarville University, School of Pharmacy, Cedarville, Ohio
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Akesha Edwards
eUniversity of Findlay, College of Pharmacy, Findlay, Ohio
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nkem Nonyel
fUniversity of Maryland Eastern Shore, School of Pharmacy, Princess Anne, Maryland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mark Sawkin
aUniversity of Missouri – Kansas City, School of Pharmacy, Kansas City, Missouri
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Objective. The purpose of this study was to identify the extent of implicit and explicit bias in a sample of pharmacy students and to determine whether there is an association between implicit bias, explicit bias, and responses to clinical cases.

Methods. Investigators sent links to two online surveys to students at six US schools and colleges of pharmacy. In the first survey, students responded to two clinical cases. Students were presented with a picture of a White or Black patient with each clinical case. On the second survey, students indicated their level of racial implicit bias as assessed by the Harvard Implicit Association Test and their level of racial explicit bias. Pearson’s correlation was used to determine the correlation between bias and responses to the clinical cases.

Results. Three hundred fifty-seven first, second, and third year pharmacy students responded to both surveys (response rate 52%). The students who were presented with the picture of a Black patient rated the patient’s pain and the reliability of the patient’s family as higher than students presented with the picture of a White patient. Students had more negative implicit and explicit bias towards Black patients. Neither implicit nor explicit bias correlated with student responses to the clinical cases.

Conclusion. Evidence of slight to moderate negative implicit bias and slight negative explicit bias towards Black patients was identified in this group of pharmacy students. Future studies that include a more representative population and heighten the stakes of the clinical scenario should be done to investigate a possible correlation between bias and clinical behaviors.

Keywords
  • bias
  • implicit
  • race
  • prejudice

INTRODUCTION

Health disparities are evident when comparing care between different races. Health disparities are defined as “…preventable differences in the burden of disease, injury, violence, or opportunities to achieve optimal health that are experienced by socially disadvantaged populations.”1 From 2014-2016, 42% of Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality measures were worse for Black patients than White patients, and these included the number of patients with an adverse event to a low molecular weight heparin, number of deaths in hospitalized patients with expected low mortality, and number of hospitalized patients with an adverse event to a hypoglycemic agent.2 Discrimination has likely resulted in these health disparities.3

Discrimination may be the result of bias, which may be explicit or implicit. Explicit bias is a situation where an individual recognizes that he or she holds negative feelings or beliefs towards one group over another or favors a certain group, unlike implicit bias, where an individual is unaware of negative feelings, beliefs, or preferences.4 Several investigators have identified an association between implicit bias and differential treatment of patients by practicing health care providers.5,6 Physicians with a pro-White bias were more likely to agree with prescribing narcotics for White patients than for Black patients.5 Physicians with a pro-White bias were also more likely to treat White patients than Black patients with thrombolytics.6 The association between implicit bias and differential treatment was seen even when the association between explicit bias and differential treatment did not exist.6 These unconscious biases may be evident to patients as well. Black patients were less likely to perceive that the clinician respected them, less likely to respect the clinician, had less confidence in the clinician, and were less likely to recommend the clinician to others when the clinician scored at a higher level of implicit positive bias for White patients.7

Other investigators, however, have not observed a correlation between bias and clinical decisions. No association was found between implicit bias and pediatrician treatment decisions regarding urinary tract infections, attention deficit disorder, and asthma.5 First year medical students were asked to rate a patient’s pain and assess the reliability of information provided by a patient’s family using clinical vignettes.8 Interestingly, students with a pro-White bias did not respond any differently to the vignettes when compared to students without bias and to students with a pro-Black bias.

Although many pharmacy school programs incorporate the topic of cultural competence/cultural sensitivity within their curricula, very few publications discuss increasing student awareness of implicit bias or discussing methods to reduce bias.9⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓⇓-18 The purpose of this study was to identify the extent of implicit and explicit bias in a sample of Doctor of Pharmacy (PharmD) students and to determine whether there is an association between implicit bias, explicit bias, and responses to clinical cases assessing patients of a different race.

METHODS

This cross-sectional study was conducted with Doctor of Pharmacy students from six schools and colleges of pharmacy within the United States. Each of the participating pharmacy schools obtained institutional review board (IRB) approval prior to participating in this study, and their IRB approval letters were submitted to the University of Missouri-Kansas City (UMKC) IRB. This study was deemed exempt by the UMKC IRB. The participating institutions and student populations were selected based on convenience sampling and interest in the project to provide a sufficient sample size to detect a correlation. Demographics of each institution are provided in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

Characteristics of Schools and Colleges of Pharmacy Participating in a Study to Identify Pharmacy Students’ Implicit and Explicit Biases Associated With Patients’ Race

A total of 688 first, second, and third year students enrolled in a social/behavioral/administrative science course within each institution’s required curriculum were invited to participate in the study from April 2019 through April 2020. A verbal consent script was read to students prior to the surveys being administered. Participants completed the survey while participating in class. Only those students who consented participated in the study. There were no consequences for not participating in the study. Participants answered questions anonymously.

The first survey collected demographic information from the subjects and asked the participants to respond to two cases. The first case was a 48-year-old male presenting to the emergency department after being assaulted. The second case was a 35-year-old female, two days postoperative presenting with symptoms suggesting delirium tremens. Investigators provided a picture of the patient for each case. Some participants received a picture of a Black patient for a case while other participants received a picture of a White patient for the same case. At one school, students were randomly assigned a picture. At the remaining schools, student names were arranged alphabetically and the students in the first half of the class received a picture of a white patient for the first case while students in the second half of the class received a picture of a Black patient for the first case. At all schools, the race of the patient was reversed for the second case. The camera angle, the distance between the patient and the camera, the patient’s facial expression, and the patient’s position (lying in bed) were the same regardless of the patient’s race. The text describing the patient’s history of present illness and the questions regarding each case were the same regardless of the race of the patient. The participants indicated the race of both patients in the first survey. The student also rated the patient’s pain for the first case on a scale of 1-10 (1 being the least pain and 10 being the worst pain imaginable) and the reliability of information provided by the patient’s family for the second case on a scale of one to five (1=very reliable, 2=reliable, 3=uncertain, 4=unreliable, 5=very unreliable). This portion of the survey was adapted from a previous study on implicit bias.8

Immediately following the first survey, investigators instructed the participants to complete the Harvard Race Implicit Association Test (IAT). The Race IAT identifies unconscious bias by measuring how long it takes an individual to group a picture of a person of a certain race with words that have either a positive or negative connotation. The theory behind the Race IAT is that those with negative bias towards a certain race will take longer to group the picture of a person of that race with a positive word and vice versa. Those completing the Race IAT are notified if they have a strong, moderate, or slight preference for White or Black people or if they have no preference. The predictive validity of the IAT has been demonstrated in previous literature.19

Immediately following completion of the Race IAT, the investigators provided the participants with a link to a second survey in which the participants indicated the results from their Race IAT, their preference for White vs Black persons (strongly prefer, moderately prefer, slightly prefer, or no preference), and how warm or cold they felt towards Black vs White persons using a scale of 0 to 10 on which 0 represented the coldest feelings, 5 represented neutral feelings, and 10 represented the warmest feelings. The Race IAT results and the explicit preferences were translated into rankings from 1 (strong preference for White people) to 4 (no preference for either race) to 7 (strong preference for Black people).

Survey responses were linked using a unique identifier code. Participants that completed both surveys and whose responses from the first survey could be linked to the second survey were included in the statistical analysis. Descriptive statistics were used to summarize the extent of implicit bias, explicit bias, and responses to clinical cases. The Mann-Whitney test was used to compare results between various subgroups of participants. Pearson correlation was used to determine the correlation between bias scores and responses to clinical cases.

RESULTS

Three hundred fifty-seven (357) participants (overall response rate 52%) responded to both surveys and provided a unique identifier code that allowed for their responses to the first survey to be linked to their responses to the second survey. Table 2 summarizes the demographic data of these respondents. The majority of students were white, female, 22-25 years of age, attending pharmacy school in the Midwest, and enrolled in the first year of pharmacy school. Two hundred seventeen subjects were presented with a picture of a Black patient and 139 subjects were presented with a picture of a White patient for the first case. The median pain rating score given by students presented with a picture of a Black patient was higher than the average pain rating score given by students presented with a picture of a White patient (7 (IQR:6-8) vs 6 (IQR:5-7), p<.05).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

Demographics of Participants in a Study to Identify Pharmacy Students’ Implicit and Explicit Biases Associated With Race

One hundred thirty-two students (132) were presented with a picture of a Black patient and 225 students were presented with a picture of a White patient for the second case. Subjects presented with a Black patient rated the information provided by the patient’s family as more reliable than the students presented with a white patient (2 [IQR:2-3]) vs 3 [IQR:3-4], p<.05).

The mean rank for Race IAT scores was 2.76, indicating that most subjects had a slight to moderate implicit preference for White people. The mean rank of explicit preference was 3.73, indicating that most subjects had a slight explicit preference for White people. The mean rankings of warmth towards Black and White people was 7.65 and 7.76 respectively, indicating most subjects had generally warm feelings for both Black and White people. The extent of implicit and explicit bias did not differ between students enrolled in their first or second year and students enrolled in their third year or between students who were shown a picture of a Black patient and students who were shown a picture of a White patient (p>.05). The extent of implicit bias and level of warmth towards European Americans and African Americans did not differ between students enrolled in an urban vs rural school (p>.05). The extent of explicit bias was statistically different between students enrolled in an urban vs rural school (p<.05), but the difference was not meaningful (median score was 4 for both urban and rural students). The extent of how warm students felt towards European Americans did not differ between Black and non-Black students. Compared to Black students, non-black students had a stronger implicit preference for European Americans (2 vs 4, p<.05), more explicit preference for European Americans (4 vs 5, p<0.05), and less warmth for African Americans (8 vs 9, p<.05).

The correlations between implicit bias, explicit bias, and responses to clinical cases are presented in Table 3. No variable, including results of the Race IAT, measures of explicit preference, measures of warmth towards White people, and measures of warmth towards Black people, demonstrated a significant correlation with ratings of pain or family reliability provided for the patient cases.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3.

Correlation Coefficients Between Clinical Cases and Measures of Bias from a Sample of Pharmacy Students from Six Schools/Colleges of Pharmacy (N=357)

DISCUSSION

While pain scores and family reliability scores were higher for participants presented with a patient case and a photograph of a Black patient than for participants presented with the same case and a photograph of a White patient, one might question the clinical significance of the differences. The differences were less than one point and it would be unlikely for a respondent noting a difference to note a change less than one point. In another study, although health professionals with more negative implicit bias towards Black patients were less likely to prescribe a narcotic for them,5 the overall pain rating was higher for the Black patient and a correlation did not exist between implicit bias and ratings of pain in this study. However, the lack of correlation between bias and pain scores, as well as bias and reliability scores, does not allow for the conclusion that health professional bias does not have an impact on patient care. Reliance on implicit bias may be stronger when the provider is faced with cases where they must make quick, difficult decisions with incomplete data.20 Because participants answered the questions anonymously in a classroom session, participating in the study was likely less stress inducing than having to make a decision regarding a real patient where the decision would have a greater impact.

Several limitations exist for this study. The results may not be generalizable to the whole pharmacy student population given differences between sample and population demographics. The sample of respondents is similar to all pharmacy schools accredited by the Accreditation Council for Pharmacy Education with respect to the proportion of female students but has a higher percentage of White students and lower percentage of Black students.21 Demographics relating to socioeconomic status and sexual orientation were not collected from the participants and may affect the level of bias. Most of the sample included first year students, so whether the results can be applied to students enrolled in later years is uncertain. The total number of participants was impacted by technical difficulties with accessing and completing the surveys at one site. Several students were unable to complete both surveys within the allotted time. No identifiable information was collected from the participants but still participants may have been compelled to provide more socially acceptable responses than truthful responses, leading to an underestimation of bias. The validity of the measures of explicit bias in pharmacy students has not been investigated. Finally, schools and colleges of pharmacy differ as to when and to what extent cultural sensitivity is taught within their curriculum, which could have an impact on the magnitude of student implicit and explicit bias and the correlation between bias and clinical decisions. Notably, despite the effect of underestimation, pro-White bias was indeed discovered. Further investigation of this bias is warranted.

CONCLUSION

Bias towards Black patients was found among a sample of pharmacy students at six US schools and colleges of pharmacy. Future studies that include a more representative student population and heighten the stakes of the clinical scenario should be done to investigate a possible correlation between bias and clinical behaviors.

  • Received February 8, 2021.
  • Accepted June 2, 2021.
  • © 2022 American Association of Colleges of Pharmacy

REFERENCES

  1. 1.↵
    Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). 2018. Health Disparities. Accessed on 12/29/2021 from https://www.cdcgov/healthyyouth/disparities/index.htm. Last reviewed on August 17, 2018
  2. 2.↵
    Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. 2017. National Healthcare Quality and Disparities Report. Rockville, MD:. AHRQ Pub. No. 18-0033-EF.
  3. 3.↵
    IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2012. How far have we come in reducing health disparities?: Progress since 2000: Workshop Summary. Washington, DC: The National Press.
  4. 4.↵
    1. Davido JF,
    2. Gaertner SL
    . Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychol Sci. 2000;11(4):315-319.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Sabin JA,
    2. Greenwald AG
    . The influence of implicit bias on treatment recommendations for 4 common pediatric conditions: Pain, urinary tract infection, attention deficit disorder, and asthma. Am J Public Health. 2012;102(5):988-995.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. 6.↵
    1. Green AR,
    2. Carney DR,
    3. Pallin DJ,
    4. Ngo LH,
    5. Raymond KL,,
    6. Iezzoni LI,
    7. Banaji MR
    . Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for black and white patients. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:1231-1238.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. 7.↵
    1. Cooper LA,
    2. Roter DL,
    3. Carson KA,
    4. Beach MC,
    5. Sabin JA,,
    6. Greenwald AG,
    7. Inui TS
    . The associations of clinicians’ implicit attitudes about race with medical visit communication and patient ratings of interpersonal care. Am J Public Health. 2012;102(5):979-987.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Haider AH,
    2. Sexton J,
    3. Sriram N,
    4. et al
    . Association of unconscious race and social class bias with vignette-based clinical assessments by medical students. JAMA. 2011;306(9):942-951.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.↵
    1. Poirier TI,
    2. Butler LM,
    3. Radhika D,
    4. Gupchup GV,
    5. Santanello C,
    6. Lynch C
    . A cultural competency course for pharmacy students. Am J Pharm Educ. 2009;73(4):Article 81.
    OpenUrl
  10. 10.↵
    1. Haack S,
    2. Phillips C
    . Teaching cultural competency through a pharmacy skills and application course series. Am J Pharm Educ. 2012;76(2):Article 27.
    OpenUrl
  11. 11.↵
    1. Westberg SM,
    2. Bumgardner MA,
    3. Lind PR
    . Enhancing cultural competency in a college of pharmacy curriculum. Am J Pharm Educ. 2005;69(5):Article 82.
    OpenUrl
  12. 12.↵
    1. Vyas D,
    2. Caligiuri FJ
    . Reinforcing cultural competency concepts during introductory pharmacy practice experiences. Am J Pharm Educ. 2010;74(7):Article 129.
    OpenUrl
  13. 13.↵
    1. Sales I,
    2. Jonkman L,
    3. Connor S,
    4. Hall D
    . A comparison of educational interventions to enhance cultural competency in pharmacy students. Am J Pharm Educ. 2013;77(4):Article 76.
    OpenUrl
  14. 14.↵
    1. Muzumdar JM,
    2. Holiday-Goodman M,
    3. Black C
    . Cultural competence knowledge and confidence after classroom activities. Am J Pharm Educ. 2010;74(8):Article 150.
    OpenUrl
  15. 15.↵
    1. Wilby KJ,
    2. Taylor J,
    3. Khalifa SI,
    4. Jorgenson D
    . A course-based cross-cultural interaction among pharmacy students in Qatar and Canada. Am J Pharm Educ. 2015;79(2):Article 26.
    OpenUrl
  16. 16.↵
    1. Mathews JL,
    2. Parkhill AL,
    3. Schlehofer DA,
    4. Starr MJ,
    5. Barnett S
    . Role-reversal exercise with deaf strong hospital to teach communication competency and cultural awareness. Am J Pharm Educ. 2011;75(3):Article 53.
    OpenUrl
  17. 17.↵
    1. Haack S
    . Engaging pharmacy students with diverse patient populations to improve cultural competence. Am J Pharm Educ. 2008;72(5):Article 124.
    OpenUrl
  18. 18.↵
    1. Avants ND,
    2. Weed E,
    3. Connelly C,
    4. et al.
    Qualitative Analysis of Student Pharmacists’ Reflection of Harvard Race Implicit Bias Association Test. Curr Pharm Teach Learn. 2018;10(5): 611-617. doi:10.1016/jcptl.2018.02.002
    OpenUrl
  19. 19.↵
    1. Greenwald AG,
    2. Uhlmann EL,
    3. Poehlman TA,
    4. Banaji MR
    . Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2009;97(1):17-41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. 20.↵
    1. Burgess DJ,
    2. van Ryn M,
    3. Crowley-Makota M,
    4. Malat J
    . Understanding the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in pain treatment: Insights from dual process models of stereotyping. Pain Med. 2006;7(2):119-134.
    OpenUrl
  21. 21.↵
    American Association of Colleges of Pharmacy. Fall 2019 Enrollments: Profile of Pharmacy Students. https://www.aacp.org/sites/default/files/2020-05/fall-2019-pps-enrollments.pdf Accessed 12/29/2021.
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education
Vol. 86, Issue 1
1 Jan 2022
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Print
Download PDF
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Correlation Between Pharmacy Students’ Implicit Bias Scores, Explicit Bias Scores, and Responses to Clinical Cases
(Your Name) has sent you a message from American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
5 + 1 =
Solve this simple math problem and enter the result. E.g. for 1+3, enter 4.
Citation Tools
Correlation Between Pharmacy Students’ Implicit Bias Scores, Explicit Bias Scores, and Responses to Clinical Cases
Jennifer Santee, Kylie Barnes, Nancy Borja-Hart, An-Lin Cheng, Juanita Draime, Akesha Edwards, Nkem Nonyel, Mark Sawkin
American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education Jan 2022, 86 (1) 8587; DOI: 10.5688/ajpe8587

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Correlation Between Pharmacy Students’ Implicit Bias Scores, Explicit Bias Scores, and Responses to Clinical Cases
Jennifer Santee, Kylie Barnes, Nancy Borja-Hart, An-Lin Cheng, Juanita Draime, Akesha Edwards, Nkem Nonyel, Mark Sawkin
American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education Jan 2022, 86 (1) 8587; DOI: 10.5688/ajpe8587
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • INTRODUCTION
    • METHODS
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • CONCLUSION
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Similar AJPE Articles

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Multi-institutional Analysis of Student and Program Variables as Predictors of Performance on the NAPLEX
  • Exploring Factors That Influence Student Engagement in Community-Engaged Learning Activities Within a Pharmacy Context
  • Self-Assessment of Therapeutic Decision-Making Skills in Pharmacy Students
Show more RESEARCH

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Keywords

  • bias
  • implicit
  • race
  • prejudice

Home

  • AACP
  • AJPE

Articles

  • Current Issue
  • Early Release
  • Archive

Instructions

  • Author Instructions
  • Submission Process
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Reviewer Instructions

About

  • AJPE
  • Editorial Team
  • Editorial Board
  • History
  • Contact

© 2022 American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education

Powered by HighWire